This summer, a number of new papers have come out with data that bear on the role of posterior parietal cortex (PPC) on perceptual decisions. First, a paper by Katz and colleagues shook things up with their new data demonstrating that pharmacological inactivation of primate PPC has little effect on perceptual decisions. These results have been talked about in the community for a while- I will hold off saying too much about them since I wrote a piece about this paper that will come out in a few weeks (Stay tuned- I’ll post a link then). But the short story is that this paper argued that despite strong modulation during perceptual decisions, primate PPC is not a member of the causal circuit for visual motion decisions.

Two papers about rodent PPC, paint a different picture. We shouldn’t be too surprised about this, since although rodent and primate PPC share the same name, they have a number of anatomical and functional differences that mean it isn’t right to think of one as the homologue of the other (in fact, could we just stop using the word, “homologue” altogether?).

The first paper, by Michael Goard and goardTaskcolleagues, measured and manipulated mouse PPC neurons during a visual detection task: the mice were shown a horizontal or vertical grating, then waited through a 3-9 second delay, then reported whether a vertical grating was present by licking a spout. The authors disrupted PPC activity optogenetically. They found that performance declined considerably when the activation took place during the time the grating was visible to the mice. Interestingly, although PPC neurons were highly active during other parts of the trial, the delay and movement periods, for instance, disruption during those times had little effect on performance. This argues that the activity during those periods may reflect signals that are computed elsewhere and fed back to PPC.

 
PrintThe second paper is from my lab. Like the Goard paper, we found that performance declined when we stimulated while animals were facing visual stimuli that they had to judge. We mainly focussed on this period, so can’t compare the results with disruption at other times. We did, however, compare disruption on visual vs. auditory trials in the same animal and the same session, and we found that effects were mostly restricted to visual decisions. This fits with the data from the paper above, and also with deficits on a visual memory task reported in mice by Chris Harvey and David Tank.

Beyond the science of our paper, it was also a landmark moment in my lab because it is our first paper not the preprint server, biorxiv! Thebiorxiv was started at Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory. It provides a way for scientists to make their work freely available to the world as they journey through the sometimes long TwitterImg2process of academic publishing. I like the idea of making the work available fast, and the fact that it is freely accessible to everyone is important too. I’m excited to be part of this new effort and…  I admit my enthusiasm prompted me to modify the rainbow unicorn of asapbio just a bit…

 

This is a guest blog written by Ashley Ashley Kyalwazi- Shot 1Kyalwazi, a participant in the Undergraduate Research Program at Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory. I am the director of the program, as well as the PI on our NSF-Funded grant (along with my bioinformatics colleague Mike Schatz) to train undergraduates in Bioinformatics and Computational Neuroscience. These fields share many mathematical ideas, such as a need for dimensionality reduction and machine learning tools, but our program is highly unusual in bringing them together. Ms Kyalwazi, one of our students funded by the program, will be a junior this upcoming year at the University of Notre Dame. Her story is below:

As I embarked on my ten-week long summer research immersion experience here at Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, I was excited. To me this was an opportunity to listen to a vast range of new ideas in major scientific disciplines, to analyze them, and then to begin forming my own. I would always carry a notepad and a pen with me around campus; I never knew what I would learn on any given day, from any given individual. All I knew, for sure, was that I would grow as a scientist and as a future physician.

Working in the systems neuroscience lab of Dr. Stephen Shea has been an incredible experience. I have had the opportunity to learn a vast new array of techniques from my mentors in the Shea lab, and to apply them as I worked on an independent project that uses a mouse model in order to gain a deeper understanding of the inhibitory network of parvalbumin neurons in the auditory cortex, and how this network regulates neural activity before, during, and after what I have come to refer to as “the maternal experience.”

The maternal experience describes any aspect of the mother-pup interaction that contributes to the context of the overall birthing process. This could range from mothers giving birth, to the act of retrieving distressed pups that find themselves isolated from the nest.  The former is an action that is characteristic to a single mother and her pups; however, the latter is one that could be translated and studied with a model incorporating virgin female mice (‘surrogates’). In my project, I was interested in understanding how maternal experience alters the neural circuitry of the surrogate’s brain, primarily focusing on a network of neurons in the cortex defined by the marker parvalbumin (PV+ cells). This network has been found to play a key role in regulating plasticity in the auditory cortex of female mice, following interaction with newborn pups (Shea et al 2016). So again I ask: what is the nature of nuture?

Last week, during journal club, my lab read a paper by Lior Cohen, Gideon Rothschild, and Adi Mizrahi titled “Multisensory Integration of Natural Odors and Sounds in the Auditory Cortex.” This paper found that neurons in A1 of mothers, and other virgin female mice, integrate pup odors and sounds, suggesting an experience-dependent model for cortical plasticity.

One of the findings that intrigued me the most as I was reading this paper, was the observation that washing the pups hindered a lactating mother’s ability to retrieve them, after they became isolated from the nest. While the authors’ emphasis on this observation was the fact that pup odor is a commanding feature of pup-retrieval behavior, I was interested in this for a slightly different reason.

To know that an act as simple as washing the pups could change the behavior of something as innate as a mother retrieving her own pups led me to wonder: is there an essential biological component of motherhood that is necessary for a pup’s development and overall survival¾ or should we, as scientists, begin to hone in on the commonalities that make up the maternal experience, and also enable virgin females to successfully retrieve isolated pups?

 

My experiments this summer utilized a combination of stereotaxic surgery (injections and craniotomies), fluorescence imaging, computer programming and image analysis in order to observe the sound-evoked spatiotemporal activity patterns in the A1 PV+ network of naïve female mice.

Blue LED light was directed through cranial windows over the left auditory cortex and recordings of eight pup calls were emitted at regular intervals for each of the 20 trials. Plotting the average intensities for the activated region of interest across the 20 trials yielded a visual representation of the GCaMP6m activation in the parvalbumin neuronal population (Table 1).

 

Table 1- Ashley

This widespread cortical GCaMP6m activation throughout the A1 PV+ neuronal network suggests that, when exposed to pup calls, female mice do not tend to differentiate among varying frequencies. Perhaps this could be due the dependency of all pups to receive this nurturing behavior, and for mothers and surrogates alike to provide it.

This suggested binary distinction of female mice¾ recognizing ‘call vs. no call,’ but not distinguishing between ‘call frequency A vs. call frequency B’¾ is one that will be further investigated in the Shea lab, as we look to hone in on the neural circuits that regulate long-term, experience-dependent plasticity in the auditory cortex.

I would like to thank my research advisor for the summer, Dr. Stephen Shea, and the directors of the Undergraduate Research Program, Dr. Anne Churchland and Kim Creteur, for providing me with this opportunity. I also thank my parents¾ Michael and Winnie Kyalwazi. Against all odds you continue to work hard and sacrifice so I have opportunities as life-changing as coming to CSHL to study neuroscience… a dream come true. Your love and support has been and will never cease to be the wind beneath my wings. It has definitely been a memorable summer for me here at Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory and I look forward to the future.

 

I attended the Neurofutures 2016 conference at the Allen Institute for Brain Sciences in Seattle last week. The conference that focussed on new technologies in the field and how they will drive new discoveries. I gave the opening plenary talk at the conference, a public lecture which you can see here. Following my lecture, I was part of a panel consisting of olfaction hero Linda Buck, blood flow guru (and recent marmoset pioneer) Alfonso Silva and eCog sage Jeff Ojemann. It was exciting to hear their take on the most exciting technologies in neuroscience. Some of the exciting new developments were highlighted by the panel included optogenetics, powerful transgenic animals (mice, marmosets and beyond) and high-throughput sequencing, just to name a few.

Screen Shot 2016-07-01 at 3.23.11 PM

I share my colleagues’ enthusiasm for those techniques, but also held fast that these techniques must be accompanied by advances in theory to support out ability to understand the incoming data. Theoretical neuroscience has historically played a fundamental role in the field as a whole, and its importance going forward cannot be understated (I have argued for this before).

A recent paper in Neuron from Kanaka Rajan, Chris Harvey and David Tank sets out to demonstrate how relatively unstructured networks can give rise to highly structured outputs that persist on slow timescales relevant to behaviors like decision-making and working memory. Such unstructured networks seem at first like exactly the wrong thing to support stimulus-driven persistent activity. Indeed, classic work in the prefrontal cortex revealed individual neurons that respond persistently during delays, presumably support the ability of the animal to hold information in mind over that delay. In mouse posterior parietal cortex, however, it’s a different story. On a memory guided decision task published previously many individual neurons respond only very transiently, for much less time than the animal holds those memories in mind. Both that paper and the current one argue that many such neurons could fire in sequence, supporting slow-timescale memory-guided decisions even in the absence of single neurons with persistent activity.

The big steps forward in the current paper are:

  1. The authors demonstrated that a randomly connected network could give rise to this activity. This was an advance for a number of reasons, including the development of a new model framework called PINning. This method builds on a now classic technique, FORCE learning which generates coherent activity patterns from chaotic networks. PINing is different because only a small percentage (~12%) of synaptic weights are allowed to change. The ability of the network to capture the complex firing rates of 437 neurons when only a few synaptic weights were allowed to change is a big deal.

    rajan

    Network that learns by PINning; red lines are the only synapses that are allowed to change during learning to match the data.

  2. The paper pointed out features of the data that are incompatible with a traditional model for persistent activity, like bump attractors. This is evidence against an appealing idea (that may be present in other systems) in which a hill of activity moves around the network, driving a persistent response.
  3. Finally, the authors found that the network’s success relied not only on the strongly choice-selective neurons you might expect, but also on neurons that weren’t selective for the animal’s choice at all. In fact, they observed that these seemingly unimportant neurons might play a critical “conveyer belt” role that was essential in supporting more difficult decisions, especially those among many alternatives. The previous paper (and indeed many other studies) mainly excluded these neurons from analysis; an understandable choice at the time, but one that now warrants reconsideration.

There is still a challenge ahead for putative mechanisms that support slow timescale behaviors like working memory and decision-making. At the moment, there are few causal manipulations that can disrupt proposed mechanisms and demonstrate an effect on behavior. In the framework here, it would be compelling to demonstrate that changing the order of the sequence changed the behavior (admittedly no small feat!). More traditional mechanisms aren’t off the hook either: demonstrating that persistent activity at the single neuron level supports working memory likewise would be aided by precise disruption experiments. Indeed, the single neuron persistence could be epiphenomenological; the persistent working memories could be supported by some other aspect of the network. Many such  manipulation experiments will be feasible in the near future.

Until then, I am excited to see a new mechanism to support slow-timescale behavior. It is counterintuitive that such network complexity can be captured by a randomly connected network, especially one in which such a small number of synapses are allowed to change.

 

 

 

Discoveries made by plant geneticists in the 1940s are changing our understanding of the brain. Specifically, Barbara McClintock’s (left) discovery of transposons, for which she won the nobel prize, has turned out to be important not only for understanding gene function in plants, but in brains as well. Tranposons, described by the New Yorker as “wandering snippets of DNA that hide in genomes, copying and pasting themselves at random” account for ~40% of our genome. They are likely to play a key role in normal brain function, and also might be involved in neurodegenerative diseases including ALS.

The importance of transposons for all biology inspired current CSHL graduate students and motivated them to create a lecture series named after Barbara McClintock. The first one was today, and in recognition of the role of transposons in the brain, they invited a neuroscientists, Ann Graybiel (right) from MIT, to be the first recipient. Ann’s work on the striatum has been critical for the field’s growing understanding of how incoming inputs can lead to actions, especially ones that are reinforced and become habitual. Her emerging work is especially exciting as her lab is leveraging modern techinques to specifically measure and manipulate classes of cells within the striatum to understand their role in different behaviors and decisions.

To commemorate the creation of this new lecture series and its first recipient, neuroscientists from around New York gathered to honor Ann and attend her talk. Researchers focussing on decision-making, attention, vision and auditory processing came together and some lively discussions ensued! It was a lot of fun to show the setups in the my lab to this crew, which included Jackie Gottlieb, Yael Niv, Heather Read and Ariana Maffei, and we realized many links between our collective research programs that I hope will lead to new collaborations down the line.

lab.001

sashank

I am happy to announce another post by a guest blogger. This time, its Sashank Pisupati, a new graduate student in my lab.

Last week, our lab read a paper by Ramon Reig & Gilad Silberberg titled “Multisensory Integration in the Mouse Striatum”. While studies of multisensory integration have focussed largely on cortical structures and the superior colliculus, this study adds to a growing body of evidence that the striatum may play a key role in this process. Striatal medium-spiny neurons (MSNs) are known to receive convergent projections from multiple sensory cortices, but relatively few studies have reported multisensory responses in these cells.

Here, the authors set out to test whether individual MSNs integrated visual (LED flashes) and tactile (whisker stimulation with air puffs) stimuli in anesthetized mice. In order to observe such synaptic integration, they performed whole-cell patch clamp recordings from striatal neurons. They targeted regions of striatum receiving projections from primary visual (V1) and somatosensory (S1) cortex, as identified by anterograde tracing using BDA.

They found sub-threshold responses to whisker stimulation (purple trace) in all the neurons they recorded from, which were modulated by stimulation strength. More interestingly, in the dorsomedial striatum a subset of these neurons were also responsive to visual stimuli (green trace), with slightly longer peak response latencies. They then presented visual and tactile stimuli together at various relative delays, and observed multisensory responses in these cells (orange & black traces) that were sublinearly additive i.e. less than the linear summation (grey traces) of the visual and tactile responses. Moreover, the peak multisensory response was maximal when the onsets/peaks of the unisensory responses were aligned, suggesting that the neurons summated congruent synaptic inputs.
gr4

These findings of multisensory cells in the mouse striatum corroborate similar reports from extracellular recordings in the striatum of rats and cats, and complement them by offering a valuable glimpse of sub-threshold activity. The sub-linear additivity described here contrasts with the super-linear additivity of firing rate responses often emphasized in studies of the superior colliculus.

One of the questions that remained at the end of our discussion, was how this result fit into models of multisensory integration such as divisive normalization4, or Bayes-optimal cue combination. While classical approaches have emphasized the degree of additivity of the unisensory responses, these models make strong predictions about how the weights assigned to each unisensory response in the summation change in accordance to the reliability of that sensory modality4. For example, we expect the contribution of a visual flash to the summation to decrease for weaker, less reliable flashes.

One could test this prediction in the author’s current setup by simply varying the stimulus strength for each modality during multisensory presentation. Combined with the power of the patch clamp approach, this could yield further insight into the sub-threshold computations being performed by these neurons, and we hope to see more such work in the future!

mattThis post is written by guest blogger, Matt Kaufman, a postdoc in my lab (left).

Last week our lab read a recent Neuron paper out of the Brody lab, by Kopec, Erlich, Brunton, Deisseroth & Brody, titled “Cortical and subcortical contributions to short-term memory for orienting movements.” This paper continues with that lab’s recent strategy of using optogenetics to briefly inactivate brain areas during decision making.

The experiments were straightforward. They trained rats to judge whether a click train was faster or slower than 50 Hz, then used optogenetics (eNpHR3.0) to inactivate either the Frontal Orienting Fields (FOF) or superior colliculus (SC) on one side of the brain at different points in the trial. This allowed Kopec et al. to see when these areas contributed to making the decision. The key experimental finding was that the rats’ decisions were most biased when either FOF or SC was silenced during the stimulus, a little less biased when silenced early in the subsequent delay, and less biased still when silenced late in the delay. Decisions were essentially unaffected when silencing was performed during the response period.

This finding is initially surprising, because tuning in the FOF increases over the course of the trial (as known from previous studies). They argue, however, that this seeming mismatch makes sense in the context of an attractor dynamics model (below). Since the evidence from the stimulus is not fluctuating in this task, the animal should be able to make his decision quickly. The increasing tuning might be due to attractor dynamics that amplify the tuning with time, while perturbations should mostly impact decisions before the neural activity has had time to settle in an attractor. Additional comparisons, including inactivating both areas together and comparing hard vs. easy trials, quantitatively fit their simple attractor model.

kopecFig

This study forms an interesting contrast with their paper from earlier this year, Hanks et al. 2015 in Nature. There, they took a similar approach but with a temporal integration task. In that task, the FOF was only critical at the end of such a stimulus. This again makes sense; you don’t want attractor dynamics if you need to integrate instead.

The question on many of our minds was: do these areas “really” exhibit attractor dynamics? On further reflection, though, this is a bit like asking whether the planets follow Newton’s laws. What I mean by that is: neurons, like orbiting planets, aren’t solving equations. Dynamical models, like Newton’s equations, are a mathematical description of how the system behaves over time. But if a model is an easy way to think about a system, and makes intuitive, useful predictions that hold up experimentally, then the model does useful work.

Many questions remain unanswered, of course. In terms of separation of function, are FOF and SC really doing the exact same thing? Are there other tasks where they would function very differently? Regarding dynamics, how does the system learn to produce these attractor dynamics? Since the FOF can apparently be trained to produce different dynamics in animals trained on different tasks, can it support either computation in an animal trained on both tasks? If so, how would it switch its dynamics? We’ll look forward to the next installment.

I recently attended a meeting as part of the Simons Collaboration on the Global Brain. A postdoc in my lab, Matt Kaufman, has an award from this group and so attended as well. The goal of the collaboration is to understand the internal neural signals that interact with sensory inputs and motor outputs to shape behavior.

It was a fantastic meeting. Blaise Aguera y Arcas (Google) talked about machine intelligence and how it has advanced dramatically in recent years, easily accomplishing tasks that seemed impossible half a decade ago. Mvl_9_hpedKVHXhWyiO8kP4aj-FYeWWbV5I7GtqmRgMAndrew Leifer (Princeton) talked about a new microscopy system for large scale imaging in c-elegans. Marlene Cohen described a surprising observation she made that the increased firing rates seen during attention are accompanied by decreased correlations among neurons.
-10RuBqErXZjaH3iLSOBfqk58xs96xYmK4WA3WZG4SEA common theme among all the presentations was the idea that understanding these internal states requires considering the activity of large neural populations. A number of analyses were put forth to achieve that. The ones that were most interesting to me are designed to compare neural population activity during different kinds on behavioral states. We began to do this in our 2014 paper (see figure 7), but have rmv09LVM5Y1vWQzNSwjrKoD5NQ6MTJEUVF9WQnrydN20eally only begun to scratch the surface. The talks and conversations at the meeting expanded our thinking about new analyses we can use to get at this question. For instance, as an animal goes form making a decision to committing to action, does the population activity simply re-scale, or does it occupy a fundamentally new space?

 

Our decisions are influenced in part by incoming sensory information, and in part by the current cognitive state of the brain. For instance, a rustle in the bushes can make you run away quickly if you are walking the dark and worrying about bears, but have little effect on your behavior if you are deep in thought about something else- your upcoming vacation, for instance. This led us to wonder, how do incoming sensory signals and ongoing cognitive signals interact to guide behavior?

A postdoc in my lab, Farzaneh Najafi, is working to understand this, supported in part by the Simons Collaboration on the Global Brain. We were fortunate to have a collaborator, John Cunningham (Columbia University) visit us today, along with a graduate student in his lab, Gamal Elsayed. Their focus is on understanding neural activity at the population level, and in particular understanding how such populations evolve over time. We hope that their approach can offer insight into our question by helping us evaluate how population dynamics differ depending on the internal cognitive state of the animal. Farzaneh, John and Gamal are pictured below. They are gathered at the 2-photon rig in our lab and are viewing neural activity of labelled inhibitory neurons.

IMG_2321

IMG_1550My lab members and I did a “Literature Blitz” today: each person in the group gave a short presentation, including only a single figure, on a recent finding in the area of decision-making and sensory guided action. Short presentations like this don’t allow for the in-depth discussions we have when we read a single paper, but they give us a snapshot of a whole field that we can absorb in just a few hours. This inevitably broadens all of our perspectives.

1.  From Kawai et al in Neuron: Motor cortex isn’t needed to support complex, sequenced movements that harvest rewards. However, motor cortex is required to learn these movements. The movies associated with this paper and incredible, definitely check them out

2. From R. Kiani et al in Neuron: Natural groupings of neurons, based on time varying response similarities, can define spatially segregated subnetworks. Surprisingly, these subnetworks have correlated noise, especially during quiet wakefulness when no stimuli are present. This approach suggests we might want to consider a new way to define cortical regions and subregions, especially in areas like the frontal lobe which has historically been difficult to parcellate.

3. From Strandbug-Peshkin at al in Science: This group fitted wild Kenyan baboons with GPS collars and worked out which factors determine their collective movements. The first part we might have guessed: the number of animals, and their commitment to a particular direction of movement play a large role in determining whether the other baboons will join the move. One aspect was surprising: although baboons have a strong social hierarchy, it doesn’t play much of a role in determine where animals go next. In other words, just because the king-of-the-pack goes north, it doesn’t mean the other baboons follow suit.

4. From Juavinett & Callaway in Current Biology: Here, the authors used intrinsic signal mapping to pinpoint multiple visual areas and then measured howthey differed in terms of their ability to represent complex motion. Specifically, they tracked whether individual neurons were sensitive to the pattern motion defined by a plaid created by two overlapping gratings. Similar to classic observations in monkey, there was a transition from primary areas which mainly reflected the pattern motion, to secondary areas (especially RL) which were more likely to respond to component motino.

5. From Chen et al in Nature Neuroscience: This paper showed that during learning of a level-press task, the spines of pyramidal neurons in primary motor cortex change dramatically. Further, they determined that this change was largely mediated by a specific class of interneurons, SOM+ neurons which preferentially target the apical dendrites of pyramidal neurons.

6. From Rohe & Noppeney in PLOS Biology: These authors used fMRI to evaluate how causal inference is performed in humans who must judge whether auditory and visual information bears on the same source. Their main observation is that this occurs hierarchically: in early sensory areas (A1 & V1), activity reflects the assumption that there are two sourcesof information, whereas in the anterior intraparietal sulcus, activity reflects the assumption that the two signals are from a common source and should be integrated.

7. From Murayama et al in Neuron: Projections from secondary motor cortex feedback to secondary somatosensory cortex to help shape information about texture in mice. This suggests that feedback projections play a key role in shaping sensory experience.

8. From Cooke et al in Nature Neuroscience: This paper coined a new term, “vidget”, which refers to a visually induced fidget. Apparently head-fixed mice are especially prone to these when they experience a novel visual stimulus, even a grating in an orientation they haven’t seen in a while. Using NMDA blockers and PV-ChR2 mice, the authors argue that memory for visual images, as evidenced by vidgets, requires area V1.

9. From M. Siegel et al in Science: Functions, such as knowledge of task context and visual responses, are shared, not compartmentalized, across cortical regions. Here. the authors recorded neurons in 6 cortical areas on a complex decision task and evaluated how representations changed from sensory to parietal to frontal regions. I liked the approach and hope the dataset will be further analyzed. By experimenting with different ways to combine neurons, the authors might learn more about the kinds of computations feasible in each area.

That’s it! Please leave comments if you’ve read these papers and have any thoughts you’d like to add. 

Fairhall lab

Computational neuroscience at the University of Washington

Pillow Lab Blog

Neural Coding and Computation Lab @ Princeton University

Churchland lab

Perceptual decision-making at Cold Spring Harbor